帳號:guest(3.144.88.35)          離開系統
字體大小: 字級放大   字級縮小   預設字形  

詳目顯示

以作者查詢圖書館館藏以作者查詢臺灣博碩士論文系統以作者查詢全國書目
作者(中文):彭 泰
作者(外文):Peng, Tai
論文名稱(中文):大學生自我控制對利他、利己資訊注意偏向的影響:自我肯定的補償作用
論文名稱(外文):The influence of college students’ self-control on altruistic and egoistic information attention bias: the compensatory effect of self affirmation
指導教授(中文):王鼎銘
指導教授(外文):Wang, Ding-Ming
口試委員(中文):林珊如
林秋斌
口試委員(外文):Lin, Shan-Ju
Lin, Chiu-Pin
學位類別:碩士
校院名稱:國立清華大學
系所名稱:學習科學與科技研究所
學號:108291466
出版年(民國):111
畢業學年度:111
語文別:中文
論文頁數:101
中文關鍵詞:大學生特質自我控制狀態自我控制利他資訊利己資訊注意偏向自我肯定
外文關鍵詞:college studentstrait self-controlstate self-controlaltruistic informationattentional biasself-affirmation
相關次數:
  • 推薦推薦:0
  • 點閱點閱:64
  • 評分評分:*****
  • 下載下載:0
  • 收藏收藏:0
自我控制是個體為了實現個人目標、社會期望或者是遵循社會道德規範嘗試自主調節行為、情緒、思想等反應的心理能力,在人類生活中扮演著重要功能。自我控制能力可分為特質自我控制和狀態自我控制,特質自我控制認為自我控制能力是個體的穩定特質,而狀態自我控制則認為個體的自我控制能力會因為個體自我控制資源的減少而表現出“自我損耗”狀態,從而造成自我控制失敗。過往研究多將兩種自我控制能力分別進行研究,尚未將兩者結合起來探討。
利他行為是人類不期望獲得自我利益,自願性地幫助他人的行為。研究發現利他行為開始於利他者會優先注意到環境中的利他資訊,再通過自我控制能力控制自身的利己衝動從而做出利他行為,因此自我控制是影響利他行為發生的一個重要因素。文獻查閱發現過去研究較少探討自我控制與利他行為之間的關係,特別是不同的自我控制條件下實驗參與者對利他、利己資訊是否具有一致的注意偏向。
從狀態自我控制的視角來看,自我控制能力會因自我控制資源的消耗而降低。個體能否通過自我肯定對自我控制資源進行補償?不同的自我肯定類型是否會對自我控制資源的補償產生差異?
基於以上問題,本研究試圖探討大學生特質自我控制能力與狀態自我控制能力對於利他、利己資訊注意偏向的影響,並進一步探討自我肯定對於自我控制資源損耗的補償作用。
本研究以中國某大學的學生為研究對象,實驗一採用2(特質自控:高vs低)×6(刺激類型:利他中性詞對同側vs利他中性詞對異側vs利己中性詞對同側vs利己中性詞對異側vs利他利己詞對同側vs利他利己詞對異側)的兩因素混合實驗設計,邀請特質自我控制能力高與低兩組各30人參與實驗,探討特質自我控制能力不同的實驗參與者對利他、利己資訊注意偏向的差異。實驗二採用2(自我損耗:高vs低)×6(刺激類型:利他中性詞對同側vs利他中性詞對異側vs利己中性詞對同側vs利己中性詞對異側vs利他利己詞對同側vs利他利己詞對異側)的兩因素混合實驗設計。邀請特質自我控制能力中等組共60人參與實驗,探討狀態自我控制資源損耗對利他、利己資訊注意偏向的影響。實驗三採用3(自我肯定類型:價值肯定vs屬性肯定vs無肯定)×6(刺激類型:利他中性詞對同側vs利他中性詞對異側vs利己中性詞對同側vs利己中性詞對異側vs利他利己詞對同側vs利他利己詞對異側)的兩因素混合實驗設計。邀請特質自我控制能力中等組共45人參與實驗,探討自我控制資源損耗後自我肯定對自我控制資源的補償作用。
研究透過三個實驗結果,得出以下結論:(1)高、低特質自我控制能力實驗參與者在利他與利己資訊注意偏向之間無顯著差異;而高、低自我控制資源損耗組之間,高自我控制資源損耗組參與者注意偏向於利己資訊。(2)對於自我肯定的補償效應實驗來說,利他中性條件下,當探測點和利他詞組同側時,價值肯定組的實驗參與者反應更快;當探測點與利他詞異側時,價值肯定組的反應時間要比無肯定組更長。實驗結果顯示特質自我控制對利他、利己資訊注意的影響較小,自我控制資源的影響較大,數據較為支援利他資訊的注意是受自我控制的處理,而對利己資訊的注意則是自動化的處理。自我肯定中的價值肯定對自我控制資源損耗後的注意偏向具有一定的補償作用。
Self-control is the psychological ability of individuals to adjust their behaviors, emotions, thoughts, and other reactions independently to achieve personal goals, social expectations, or follow social moral norms, which plays an important role in human life. Previous studies mostly emphasized subdivided self-control into trait self-control oriented towards stable personality traits and state self-control from the perspective of limited self-control resources.
Altruistic behavior is the behavior of helping others voluntarily which unintended gain self-interest benefits. Research studies suggest that before individuals complete altruistic behaviors, altruists need to pay attention to the altruistic information in the environment, and then control their egoistic impulses through self-control to make altruistic behaviors. That is to say, self-control is an important factor which affecting the occurrence of altruistic behaviors. However, few studies have paid attention to the relationship between self-control and altruistic behavior, especially whether experimental participants have a consistent attentional bias to altruistic and egoistic information under different self-control conditions.
From the perspective of state self-control, self-control ability would be reduced by the consumption of self-control resources. Can individuals compensate for self-control resources through self-affirmation? Do different types of self-affirmation make a difference in the compensation of self-control resources?
This study aims to explore the influence of college students' trait self-control ability and state self-control ability on altruistic and egoistic information attentional bias, and further explore the compensatory effect of self-affirmation on the depletion of self-control resources.
In this study, students who come from a university in China were selected as the sample, the College Students Self-Control Ability Scale was used for testing, and two groups with high and low trait self-control abilities were screened out. According to the altruistic, egoistic, and neutral word materials selected by the material selection questionnaire, behavioral experiments were designed. Experiment 1 adopted a two-factor mixed experimental design of 2 (trait self-control: high vs low) × 6 (stimulation type: altruistic neutral words on the same side vs altruistic neutral words on the opposite side vs egoistic neutral words on the same side vs egoistic neutral words on the opposite side vs altruistic neutral words on the same side vs egoistic neutral words on the opposite side), and invited 30 subjects in each group of high and low trait self-control to participate in the experiment, to explore the experimental participation of different trait self-control Differences in the attentional bias of altruistic and egoistic information. Experiment 2 adopted a two-factor mixed experimental design of 2 (ego depletion: high vs low) × 6 (stimulation type: altruistic neutral words on the same side vs altruistic neutral words on the opposite side vs egoistic neutral words on the same side vs egoistic neutral words on the opposite side vs altruistic neutral words on the same side vs egoistic neutral words on the opposite side). A total of 60 subjects in the medium group of trait self-control were invited to participate in the experiment to explore the effect of state self-control resource depletion on altruistic and egoistic information attentional bias. Experiment 3 adopted a two-factor mixed experimental design of 3 (self-affirmation type: value affirmation vs attribute affirmation vs no affirmation) × 6 (stimulation type: altruistic neutral words on the same side vs altruistic neutral words on the opposite side vs egoistic neutral words on the same side vs egoistic neutral words on the opposite side vs altruistic neutral words on the same side vs egoistic neutral words on the opposite side). A total of 45 subjects in the trait self-control medium group were invited to participate in the experiment to explore the compensatory effect of self-affirmation on self-control resources after the depletion of self-control resources.
The results of three experiments lead to the following conclusions: (1) No significant difference was found between the altruistic and egoistic information attentional biases in the high and low trait self-control experiment participants; while the high and low self-control resource depletion groups In between, participants in the high self-control resource depletion group paid attention to self-interested information. (2) For the self-affirmation compensation effect experiment, under altruistic neutral conditions, when the probe point and the altruistic phrase were on the same side, the participants in the value affirmation group responded faster; when the probe point was on the opposite side of the altruistic phrase, the reaction time of the value affirmation group was longer than that of the no affirmation group. These findings indicate that trait self-control has little effect on attention to altruistic and egoistic information, while self-control resources have a greater effect. The data supports that attention to altruistic information is processed by self-control, while attention to self-interested information is automated. deal with. Value affirmation in self-affirmation has a certain compensatory effect on attentional bias after the depletion of self-control resources.

摘要 i
Abstract iv
致謝 vi
表目錄 x
圖目錄 xi
第一章 緒論 1
第一節 研究背景與動機 1
第二節 研究目的與問題 4
第三節 研究範圍與限制 5
第四節 名詞解釋 6
第五節 研究流程圖 8
第二章 文獻回顧 9
第一節 自我控制的概念及相關研究概述 9
第二節 利他行爲的界定以及相關研究 18
第三節 注意偏向的界定及相關研究 21
第四節 自我肯定的概念及相關研究概述 26
第五節 自我控制對利他行爲注意偏向的相關研究以及自我肯定的作用 32
第三章 研究方法 36
第一節 各研究變項之間的關係 36
第二節 研究總架構 37
第三節 實驗一:特質自控對利他、利己資訊注意偏向的影響 39
第四節 實驗二:自我損耗對利他、利己資訊注意偏向的影響 46
第五節 實驗三:自我損耗與利他、利己資訊注意偏向:自我肯定的補償作用 53
第四章 研究結果 60
第一節 實驗一:特質自控與利他利己資訊注意偏向的影響 60
第二節 實驗二:自我損耗與利他、利己資訊注意偏向的影響 64
第三節 實驗三:自我損耗與利他、利己資訊注意偏向:自我肯定的補償作用 69
第四節 研究發現與討論 73
第五章 研究結論與研究建議 78
第一節 研究結論 78
第二節 研究建議 81
參考文獻 83
一、中文文獻 83
二、西文文獻 86
附錄 95
附錄一:大學生特質自我控制量表 95
附錄二:用於材料選擇問卷設計的利他、利己、中性詞語材料 98
附錄三:利他、利己、中性詞語材料選擇問卷(節選) 99
附錄四:經過挑選最終用於行為實驗的詞語材料 101
一、中文文獻
于斌, 樂國安, 劉惠軍. (2013). 自我控制的力量模型. 心理科學進展, 21, 1272–1282.
王小金. 自動化情緒調節對利他行為和注意偏向的影響. (Doctoral dissertation, 南京師範大學).
任俊, 李瑞雪, 詹鋆, 劉迪, 林曼, 彭年強. (2014). 好人可能做出壞行為的心理學解釋——基於自我控制資源 損耗的研究證據. 心理學報, 46, 841–851.
任彧, 郝芳. (2018). 人際距離和情緒知覺對利他選擇的影響. 心理與行為研究, 16(3), 321-326.
何垚, & 黃希庭. (2012). 自我肯定的潛在機制與影響因素. 西南大學學報(社會科學版), 38(2), 53-6.
何甯,朱雲莉. (2016). 自愛與他愛:自戀、共情與內隱利他的關係. 心理學報, 48(2), 199-210.
宋洪波, 符明秋, 楊帥. (2015). 活力: 一個歷久彌新的研究課題. 心理科學進展, 23, 1668–1678.
李海虹, 尚思源, 謝曉非. (2022). 利他行爲的遺傳基礎:來自定量遺傳學和分子遺傳學的證據. 心理科學進展, 30, 1-15.
周淑金, 李奧斯卡, 羅俊龍. (2016). 發散性思維與幽默的認知神經機制比較——基於雙加工理論的視角. 心理科學,(6), 1525-1530.
金國敏,李丹. (2020). 慈悲冥想對利他行爲的影響及其認知神經機制. 心理科學進展, 28, 978-986.
紀麗燕, 陳寧軒, 丁錦紅, & 魏萍. (2015). 獎賞預期調節局部注意干擾效應. 心理學報, 47(6), 721-733.
胡心怡,陳英和.(2017).自我肯定方式降低高威脅後的消極情緒. 心理科學,40(1),174-180.
胡聖楠, 成志娟, 竇東徽,張紅川, 翁學東. (2018). 他人積極情緒對自我控制資源的替代恢復及機制. 心理與行為研究,16(1), 45-50.
寇彧, 付豔, 馬豔. (2004). 初中生認同的親社會行為的初步研究. 心理發展與教育, 20(4), 43-48.
張孟可, 李晴, 尹首航, & 陳安濤. (2021). 衝突水準的變化誘發衝突適應. 心理學報, 53(2), 11.
張麗華,李娜,劉婕,代嘉幸.(2018).情緒啟動對低自尊個體注意偏向的影響.心理與行為研究, 16(6), 763-770.
陳園園. 學習能力的社會比較、自我肯定對親社會行為傾向影響的實驗研究. (Doctoral dissertation, 西南大學).
費定舟, 錢東海, 黃旭辰. (2016). 利他行為的自我控制過程模型:自我損耗下的道德情緒的正向作用. 心理學報,48(9), 1175-1183.
陽海英, 張力為, 惠悲荷, 陸雯, & 張連成. (2015). 認知失調與大學生運動員自我損耗:自我肯定的預防作用. 體育科學, 35(5), 29-37.
黃雨晴. (2012). 群際威脅背景中低社會經濟地位群體的注意偏好研究. (Master's thesis, 西南大學).
黃麗, 楊廷忠, 季忠民. (2003). 正性負性情緒量表的中國人群適用性研究.中國心理衛生雜誌(1), 54-56.
楊眾望, 吳佳敏. (2020). 利他,利己資訊注意偏向抑制與自我損耗. 心理技術與應用, 8(3), 8.
董軍, 付淑英, 盧山, 楊紹峰, 齊春輝. (2018). 自我控制失敗的理論模型與神經基礎. 心理科學進展, 26(1),134-143.
潘偉剛. 特質自我控制的神經機制. (Doctoral dissertation, 西南大學).
蔣達, 王歆睿, 傅麗, 周仁來. (2008). 內隱利他行為的實驗研究. 心理科學,31(1), 79-82.
黎建斌,聶衍剛,曾敏霞,竇凱. (2012). 自我控制失敗的心理因素、神經表達及交互作用芻議. 華東師範大學學報(教育科學版), 30(4), 44-50.
聶衍剛, 劉莉, 曾燕玲, & 寧志軍. (2015). 道德自我對利己和利他行為傾向的注意偏向. 心理與行為研究, 13(5), 678-683.
羅禹, 念靖晴, 鮑未, 張靜靜, 趙守盈, & 潘運等. (2020). 急性應激損害對威脅刺激的注意解除. 心理學報, 52(1), 12.
譚樹華, 許燕, 王芳, 宋婧. (2012). 自我損耗: 理論, 影響因素及研究走向. 心理科學進展, 20, 715–725
譚樹華, 郭永玉. (2008). 大學生自我控制量表的修訂. 中國臨床心理學雜誌, 16(5), 468-470.
鐘華,郭永玉.(2008).利他人格研究述評. 華東師範大學學報(教育科學版),26(1),68-73.
鐘毅平, 陳智勇, 羅西, 王錫愛, & 易文婷. (2014). 自我肯定對自尊及自我評價的影響. 中國臨床心理學雜誌, 22(3), 5. 
二、西文文獻
Alberts, H. J., Martijn, G., &Vries, N. K. (2011). Fighting self-control failure: Overcoming ego depletion by increasing self-awareness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 58–62.
Algom, D., Chajut, E., & Lev, S. (2004). A rational look at the emotional stroop phenomenon: A generic slowdown, not a stroop effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 323–338.
Allport, G. W., Vernon, P. E., & Lindzey, G. (1951). Study of values: A scale for measuring the dominant interests in personality. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
American Psychological Association. (2012). What you need to know about willpower: The psychological science of self-control. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Arneklev, B. J., Grasmick, H. G., Tittle, C. R., & Bursik Jr, R. J. (1993). Low self-control and imprudent behavior. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 9(3), 225-247.
Batson, C. D (1987). Prosocial motivation: is it ever truly altruistic? Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,20, 65-122.
Barbara, A., Eriksen C., W., & Eriksen. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a non-search task. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143-149.
Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1994). Losing control: How and why people fail at self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic press.
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource?.J Pers Soc Psychol,74(5), 1252-1265.
Baumeister, R. F, Heatherton, T. F, & Tice, D. M. (1995). Losing control, how and why people fail at self-regulation.Clinical Psychology Review,15(4), 367-368.
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 351-355.
Bean, C. G., Winefield, H. R., Sargent, C., & Hutchinson, A. D. (2015). Differential associations of job control components with both waist circumference and body mass index. Social Science & Medicine, 143, 1–8.
Botvinick, M., & Braver, T. (2015). Motivation and Cognitive Control: From Behavior to Neural Mechanism. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 83-113.
Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends in cognitive sciences, 16(2), 106-113.
Braver, T. S., Paxton, J. L., Locke, H. S., & Barch, D. M. (2009). Flexible neural mechanisms of cognitive control within human prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(18), 7351-7356.
Bushman, B. J., DeWall, C. N., Pond, R. S., Jr., & Hanus, M. D. (2014). Low glucose relates to greater aggression in married couples. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 6254–6257.
C, MacLeod, A, Mathews, P, & Tata. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of abnormal psychology, 95(1), 15–20.
Carlson, M., Charlin, V., & Miller, N. (1988). Positive mood and helping behavior: A
test of six hypotheses. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 55(2), 211-229.
Carter, E. C., & McCullough, M. E. (2013). Is ego depletion too incredible? Evidence for the overestimation of the depletion effect. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 683–684.
Carter, E. C., & McCullough, M. E. (2014). Publication bias and the limited strength model of self-control: Has the evidence for ego depletion been overestimated. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 823.
Carter, E. C., Kofler, L. M., Forster, D. E., & McCullough, M. E. (2015). A series of meta-analytic tests of the depletion effect: Self-control does not seem to rely on a limited resource. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 796–815.
Čehajić-Clancy, S., Effron, D. A., Halperin, E., Liberman, V., & Ross, L. D. (2011). Affirmation, acknowledgment of in-group responsibility, group-based guilt, and support for reparative measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 256–270.
Churchill, S., Jessop, D. C., Green, R., & Harris, P. R., (2018). Self-affirmation improves self-control over snacking among participants low in eating self-efficacy. Appetite, 123, 264-268.
Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 203-216.
Creswell, J. D., Welch, W., Taylor, S. E., Sherman, D. K., Gruenewald, T., & Mann, T. (2005). Affirmation of personal values buffers Neuroendocrine and psychological stress responses. Psychological Science, 16, 846–851.
Creswell, J. D., Dutcher, J. M., Klein, W. M. P., Harris, P. R., & Levine, J. M.
(2013). Self-affirmation improves problem-solving under stress. PLOS ONE, 8.
Crocker, J., Niiya, Y., Mischkowski, D. (2008). Why does writing about important values reduce defensiveness? Self-affirmation and the role of positive other-directed feelings. Psychological Science, 19(7), 740-747.
Crocker, J., Canevello, A., & Brown, A. A. (2017). Social motivation: Costs and benefits of selfishness and otherishness. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 299–325.
Cohen, G. L., Aronson, J., Steele, C. M. (2000). When beliefs yield to evidence: Reducing biased evaluation by affirming the self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,26, 1151–1164.
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Purdie-Vaugns, V., Apfel, N., & Brzustoski, P. (2009). Recursive processes in self-affirmation: Intervening to close the minority achievement gap. Science, 324, 400−403.
Cohen, G. L., Aronson, J., & Steele, C. M. (2016). When beliefs yield to evidence: reducing biased evaluation by affirming the self. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(9), 1151-1164.
Correll, J., Spencer, S. J., & Zanna, M. P. (2004). An affirmed self and an open mind: Self-affirmation and sensitivity to argument strength. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 350–356.
Cook, J. E., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., & Cohen, G. L. (2012). Chronic threat and contingent belonging: Protective benefits of values affirmation on identity development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 479−496.
Darley, J. M., & Latane, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: diffusion of responsibility. J Pers Soc Psychol, 8(4, Pt.1), 377-383.
Danziger, S., Levav, J., & Avnaim-Pesso, L. (2011). Extraneous factors in judicial decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 6889–6892.
De Ridder, D. T., de Boer, B. J., Lugtig, P., Bakker, A. B., & van Hooft, E. A. (2011). Not doing bad things is not equivalent to doing the right thing: Distinguishing between inhibitory and initiatory self-control. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(7), 1006-1011.
De Ridder, D. T., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Taking stock of self-control a meta-analysis of How trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(1), 76-99.
Derrick, J. L. (2013). Energized by television familiar fictional worlds restore self-control. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 299–307.
Dovidio, J. F. (1984). Helping Behavior and altruism: An empirical and conceptual overview. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 17(4), 361-427.
Duckworth, A. L., & Kern, M. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(3), 259-268.
Ent, M. R., Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (2015). Trait self-control and the avoidance of temptation. Personality and Individual Differences, 74, 12-15.
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & psychophysics, 16(1), 143-149.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175-191.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149-1160.
Fenske, M. J., & Eastwood, J. D. (2003). Modulation of focused attention by faces expressing emotion: Evidence from flanker tasks. Emotion, 3(4), 327-343.
Freud, S. (1930/1961). Civilization and its discontents (Standard ed.). London: Hogarth Press.
Friese, M., & Wänke, M. (2014). Personal prayer buffers self-control depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 51, 56–59.
Funder, D. C., Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1983). Delay of gratification: some longitudinal personality correlates. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 44(44), 1198-1213.
Farrelly, D., Clemson,P.,& Guthrie,M. (2016).Are Women’s Mate Preferences for Altruism Also Influenced by Physical Attractiveness?Evolutionary Psychology,5,1-6.
Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). The physiology of willpower: Linking blood glucose to self-control. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 303–327.
Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 325–336.
Gailliot, M. T., Schmeichel, B. J., &Baumeister, R. F. (2006). Self-regulatory processes defend against the threatof death: Effects of self-control depletion and traitself-control on thoughts and fears of dying. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 49–62.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (2014). Weakness of the will: is a quick fix possible? Motivation and Emotion, 38(3), 305-322.
Govorun, O., & Payne, B. K. (2006). Ego-depletion and prejudice: Separating automatic and controlled components. Social Cognition, 24, 111–136.
Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2001). Health of the implicit association test at age 3. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48(2), 85-93.
Gu, R., Yang, J., Shi, Y., Luo, Y., Luo, Y. L. L., &Cai, H. (2016). Be strong enough to say no: self-affirmation increases rejection to unfair offers. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1-9.
Hart, Blanton, and, Brett, W., & Pelham, et al. (2001). Overconfidence as dissonance reduction - sciencedirect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37(5), 373-385.
Harris, P., & Miles, E. (2015). The impact of self-affirmation on working memory and self-control. Bulletin of the European Health Psychology Society, 17.
Harris, P. S., Harris, P. R., & Miles, E. (2017). Self-affirmation improves performance on tasks related to executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 281-285.
Hoffman, L., M. (1975). Developmental synthesis of affect and cognition and its implications for altruistic motivation. Developmental Psychology, 11(5), 607-622.
Hofmann, W., Friese, M., & Strack, F. (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(2), 162-176.
Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R. W., & Schmitt, M. (2008). Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behavior: Toward an individual differences perspective on behavior determination by automatic versus controlled processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 962–977.
Hoffman, M. L. (2001). Prosocial behavior and empathy: Developmental processes. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 12230-12233.
Howell, A. J. (2017). Self-affirmation theory and the science of well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 18(1), 1-19.
Hui, B. P. H., Ng, J. C. K., Berzaghi, E., Cunningham-Amos, L. A., & Kogan, A. (2020). Rewards of kindness? A meta-analysis of the link between prosociality and well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 146, 1084–1116.
Imhoff, R., Schmidt, A. F., & Gerstenberg, F. (2014). Exploring the interplay of trait self‐control and ego depletion: empirical evidence for ironic effects. European Journal of Personality, 28(5), 413-424.
Inzlicht, M., McKay, L., & Aronson, J. (2006). Stigma as ego depletion how being the target of prejudice affects self-control. Psychological Science, 17, 262–26.
Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 450-463.
Inzlicht, M., & Gutsell, J. N. (2010). Running on empty: neural signals for self-control failure. Psychological Science, 18(11), 933-937.
Inzlicht, M., Schmeichel, B. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2014). Why self-control seems (but may not be) limited. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(3), 127-133.
Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego depletion-Is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological Science of the United States of America, 21, 1686–1693.
Jovev, M., Chanen, A., Green, M., Cotton, S., Proffitt, T., & Coltheart, M., et al. (2011). Emotional sensitivity in youth with borderline personality pathology. Psychiatry Research, 187(1-2), 0-240.
Kaiser, K. (2017). A new taxonomy of altruism in terms of prosocial behaviors. Dialogue & Nexus, 4, article 4.
Kamboj, S. K., Place, H., Barton, J. A., Linke, S., Curran, H. V., & Harris, P. R. (2016). Processing of alcohol-related health threat in at-risk drinkers: an online study of gender-related self-affirmation effects. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 51(6), 756-762.
Kohn, N., Eickhoff, S., Scheller, M., Laird, A., Fox, P., & Habel, U. (2014). Neural network of cognitive emotion regulation—an ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis. Neuroimage, 87, 345-355.
Kurzban, R. (2010). Does the brain consume additional glucose during self-control tasks? Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 244–259.
Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: a developmental perspective. Develop Psychol, 18(2), 199-214.
Li, H., Song, Y., & Xie, X. (2020). Altruistic or selfish? Responses when safety is threatened depend on childhood socioeconomic status. European Journal of Social Psychology, 50, 1001–1016.
Li, J. B., Nie, Y. G., Zeng, M. X., Huntoon, M., & Smith, J. L. (2013). Too exhausted to remember: ego depletion undermines subsequent event-based prospective memory. International Journal of Psychology, 48(6), 1303-1312.
Lindsay, E. K., & Creswell, J. D. (2014). Helping the self-help others: self-affirmation increases self-compassion and pro-social behaviors. Frontiers in
Psychology, 5, 421.
Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users' guide to the stop signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189–239). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Maner, J. K., Gailliot, M. T., & Dewall, C. N. (2007). Adaptive attentional attunement: evidence for mating-related perceptual bias. Evolution & Human Behavior, 28(1), 28-36.
McQueen, A., & Klein, W. M. P. (2006). Experimental manipulations of self-affirmation: A systematic review. Self and Identity, 5, 289–354.
Muraven, M., Shmueli, D., & Burkley, E. (2006). Conserving self-control strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(3), 524-537.
Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychological review, 106(1), 3-19.
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. I. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244(4907), 933-938.
Miller, A. K. (2010). Examining the errors and self-corrections on the Stroop Test. Unpublished master’s thesis, Cleveland State University.
Michelle, Lee, Roz, & Shafran. (2008). Processing biases in eating disorders: the impact of temporal factors. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 41(4), 372-375.
Marleen, G. (2018). The 'operational' definition of self-control. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1231.
Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychological review, 106(1), 3-19. Molden, D. C., Hui, C. M., Scholer, A. A., Meier, B. P., Noreen, E. E., D’Agostino, P. R., & Martin, V. (2012). Motivational versus metabolic effects of carbohydrates on self-control. Psychological Science, 23, 1137–1144.
Mischel, W. (1961). Preference for delayed reinforcement and social responsibility. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 62(1), 1-7.
Mischel, W., & Gilligan, C. (1964). Delay of gratification, motivation for the prohibited gratification, and responses to temptation. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 411–417.
Mischel, W., & Metzner, R. (1962). Preference for delayed reward as a function of age, intelligence, and length of delay interval. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 64(6), 425.
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. J Pers Soc Psychol, 54(4), 687-696.
Muraven, M. (2010a). Building self-control strength: practicing self-control leads to improved self-control performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(2), 465-468.
Muraven, M., & Slessareva, E. (2003). Mechanisms of self-control failure: Motivation and limited resources. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 894–906.
Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2006). Improved self-control: the benefits of a regular program of academic study. Basic & Applied Social Psychology, 28(1), 1-16.
Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2007). Improvements in self-control from financial monitoring. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28(4), 487-501.
Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the
snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 130(3), 466-
478.
Osgood, J. M., & Muraven, M. (2015). Self-control depletion does not diminish attitudes about being prosocial but does diminish prosocial behaviors. Basic & Applied Social Psychology, 37(1), 68-80.
Patton, J., Stanford, M., & Barratt, E. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of clinical psychology, 51(6), 768-774.
Pereda, M., Brana-Garza, P., Rodríguez-Lara, I., & Sánchez, A. (2017). The emergence of altruism as a social norm. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 9684.
Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 109(2), 160-174.
Reed, M. B., & Aspinwall, L. G. (1998). Self-affirmation reduces biased processing of health-risk information. Motivation & Emotion, 22(2), 99-132.
Ruci, L., Allen, Z. M. V., & Zelenski, J. M. (2018). Pro-social personality traits, helping behavior, and ego-depletion: is helping really easier for the dispositionally pro-social? Personality & Individual Differences, 120, 32-39.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Self-regulation and the problem of human autonomy: Does psychology need choice, self-determination, and will? Journal of Personality, 74, 1557–1586.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008). From ego depletion to vitality: Theory and findings concerning the facilitation of energy available to the self. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 702–717.
Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality, and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. Journal of Personality, 65, 529–565.
Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, I., Bransome Jr, E. D., & Beck, L. H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of consulting psychology, 20(5), 343-350.
Sara, Kim, Ann, L, & McGill. (2017). Helping others by first affirming the self: when self-affirmation reduces ego-defensive downplaying of others' misfortunes. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin.
Schimel, J., Arndt, J., Banko, K. M., & Cook, A. (2004). Not all self-affirmations were created equal: the cognitive and social benefits of affirming the intrinsic (vs. extrinsic) self. Social Cognition, 22(1), 75-99.
Schmeichel, B. J., Vohs, K. (2009). Self-affirmation and self-control: affirming core values counteracts ego depletion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 770-782.
Schumann, & Karina. (2014). An affirmed self and a better apology: the effect of
self-affirmation on transgressors" responses to victims. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 54, 89-96.
Sherman, D. K., & Cohen, G. L. (2006). The psychology of self‐defense: self‐affirmation theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38(06), 183-242.
Sherman, D. K., & Hartson, K. A. (2011). Reconciling self-protection with self-improvement: Self-affirmation theory. In M. D. Alicke & C. Sedikides (Eds.), Handbook of self-enhancement and self-protection (pp. 128–151). New York City. The Guilford Press.
Sherman, D. K., & Hartson, K. A. (2013). Deflecting the Trajectory and Changing the Narrative: How Self-Affirmation Affects Academic Performance and Motivation Under Identity Threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 591–618.
Sherman, D., Nelson, L. D., & Steele, C. M. (2000). Do messages about health risks threaten the self? increasing the acceptance of threatening health messages via self-affirmation. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(9), 1046-1058.
Shin, M., Y Kim, & Park, S. (2020). Effect of psychological distance on intention in self-affirmation theory. Psychological Reports, 123(6), 2101-2124.
Simon, J., & Rudell, A. (1967). Auditory SR compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. The Journal of applied psychology, 51(3), 300-304.
Spencer, S. J., Logel, C., & Davies, P. G. (2015). Stereotype threat. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1),415-437.
Stapel, Diederik, A., Linde, V. D., & Lonneke, A. J. G. (2011). What drives self-affirmation effects? on the importance of differentiating value affirmation and attribute affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(1), 34-45.
Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: sustaining the integrity of the self. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 261–302.
Steele, C. M., & Liu, T. J. (1983). Dissonance processes as self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(1), 5−19.
Sternberg, S. (1969). Memory-scanning: mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 57(4), 421-457.
Stroop, J. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643-662.
Stillman, T. F., Tice, D. M., Fincham, F. D., Lambert, N. M. (2009) The psychological presence of family improves self-control. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,28(4),498-529.
Schmeichel, B. J., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Intellectual performance and ego depletion: Role of the self in logical reasoning and other information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 33–46.
Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self‐control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of personality, 72(2), 271-324.
Taber, J. M., Howell, J. L., Emanuel, A. S., Klein, W. M., Ferrer, R. A., & Harris, P. R. (2015). Associations of spontaneous self-affirmation with health care experiences and health information seeking in a national survey of us adults. Psychology & Health, 31(3), 1.
Tian, A. D., Schroeder, J., Häubl, G., Risen, J. L., Norton, M. I., & Gino, F. (2018). Enacting rituals to improve self-control. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 114(6), 851-876.
Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self:positive affect helps improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(3), 379-384.
Thomaes, S., Bushman, B. J., De Castro, B. O., & Reijntjes, A. (2012). Arousing
“gentle passions” in young adolescents: sustained experimental effects of value affirmations on prosocial feelings and behaviors. Developmental
Psychology,48(1), 103-110.
Tornero, E., Sánchez-Romera, J. F., Morosoli, J. J., Vázquez, A., & Ordoñana, J. R.
(2017). Altruistic behavior among twins: Willingness to fight and self-sacrifice
for their closest relatives. Human Nature, 29(1), 1-12.
van Koningsbruggen, G. M., Das, E., Roskosewoldsen, D. R. (2009). How self-affirmation reduces defensive processing of threatening health information: evidence at the implicit level. Health Psychology Official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology American Psychological Association, 28(5), 563-568.
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., & Ciarocco, N. J. (2005). Self-regulation and self-presentation: Regulatory resource depletion impairs impression management and effortful self-presentation depletes regulatory resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 632–657.
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. M., Nelson, N. M., & Tice, D. M. (2014). Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: A limited- resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. Motivation Science, 1(Suppl.), 19–42.
Wang, X., & Zhao, X. (2016). The mediating role of temporal considerations on the effects of self-affirmation on responses to organ donation messages. Health Communication, 1-8.
Wang, X. T., & Dvorak, R. D. (2010). Sweet future: Fluctuating blood glucose levels affect future discounting. Psychological Science, 21, 183–188.
Wang, Y., Ge, J., Zhang, H., Wang, H., & Xie, X. (2020). Altruistic behaviors relieve physical pain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117, 950–958.
Wilson, D. S., & Wilson, E. O. (2007). Rethinking the theoretical foundation of sociobiology. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 82, 327–348.
Xu, A. J., Schwarz, N., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (2015). Hunger promotes acquisition of nonfood objects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112, 2688–2692.
Yusoff, N., Grüning, A., & Browne, A. (2011). Modelling the Stroop effect: Dynamics in inhibition of automatic stimuli processing. Advances in Cognitive Neurodynamics (II) (pp. 641–645). Netherlands: Springer
 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top
* *