|
Aaronson, D., & Scarborough, H. S. (1976). Performance theories for sentence coding: Some quantitative evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance, 2(1), 56. Agnew, Z. K., van de Koot, H., McGettigan, C., & Scott, S. K. (2014). Do sentences with unaccusative verbs involve syntactic movement? Evidence from neuroimaging. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(9), 1035-1045. Barber, H., & Carreiras, M. (2005). Grammatical gender and number agreement in Spanish: An ERP comparison. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(1), 137-153. Bornkessel, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2006). The extended argument dependency model: a neurocognitive approach to sentence comprehension across languages. Psychological review, 113(4), 787. Cheng, L. L.-S. (1989). Transitivity alternations in Mandarin Chinese. Proceedings of the third Ohio state University conference on Chinese Linguistics, Cheng, L. L.-S., & Huang, J. (1994). On the argument structure of resultative compounds. Chow, W.-Y., & Phillips, C. (2013). No semantic illusions in the “Semantic P600” phenomenon: ERP evidence from Mandarin Chinese. Brain research, 1506, 76-93. Chow, W.-Y., Smith, C., Lau, E., & Phillips, C. (2016). A “bag-of-arguments” mechanism for initial verb predictions. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(5), 577-596. Chow, W. Y., Lau, E., Wang, S. P., & Phillips, C. (2018). Wait a second! delayed impact of argument roles on on-line verb prediction [Article]. Language Cognition and Neuroscience, 33(7), 803-828. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2018.1427878 Coulson, S., King, J. W., & Kutas, M. (1998). Expect the unexpected: Event-related brain response to morphosyntactic violations. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13(1), 21-58. Dambacher, M., Kliegl, R., Hofmann, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2006). Frequency and predictability effects on event-related potentials during reading. Brain research, 1084(1), 89-103. Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis [Article]. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 Fellbaum, C., Zribi-Hertz, A., & Indiana University Linguistics, C. (1989). The middle construction in French and English : a comparative study of its syntax and semantics. Indiana University Linguistics Club. Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78-84. https://doi.org/Doi 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01839-8 Friederici, A. D. (2006). The neural basis of sentence processing: Inferior frontal and temporal contributions. Broca’s region, 196-217. Friederici, A. D., & Frisch, S. (2000). Verb argument structure processing: The role of verb-specific and argument-specific information [Article]. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(3), 476-507. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2709 Friedmann, N., Taranto, G., Shapiro, L. P., & Swinney, D. (2008). The leaf fell (the leaf): The online processing of unaccusatives. Linguistic inquiry, 39(3), 355-377. Gorrell, P. (1998). Syntactic analysis and reanalysis in sentence processing. In Reanalysis in sentence processing (pp. 201-245). Springer. Grodzinsky, Y., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). Neuroimaging of syntax and syntactic processing. Current opinion in neurobiology, 16(2), 240-246. Gunter, T. C., Friederici, A. D., & Schriefers, H. (2000). Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(4), 556-568. Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(4), 439-483. Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D. (1999). Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: Early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 194-205. Her, O.-S. (2007). Argument-function mismatches in Mandarin resultatives: A lexical mapping account. Lingua, 117(1), 221-246. Hoekstra, T., & Roberts, I. (1993). Middle constructions in Dutch and English. In Knowledge and language (pp. 183-220). Springer. Hoshi, H. (1994). Theta-role assignment, passivization, and excorporation. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 3(2), 147-178. Huang, C.-R., Kilgarriff, A., Wu, Y., Chiu, C.-M., Smith, S., Rychlý, P., Bai, M.-H., & Chen, K.-J. (2005). Chinese Sketch Engine and the extraction of grammatical collocations. Proceedings of the fourth SIGHAN workshop on Chinese language processing, Huang, J. C.-T., & Liu, N. (2014). A new passive form in Mandarin: Its syntax and implications. International Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 1(1), 1-34. Jackson, C. N., Lorimor, H., & van Hell, J. G. (2020). The ERP correlates of thematic role assignment for passive versus active sentences [Article]. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 54, 13, Article 100886. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100886 Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: from eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological review, 87(4), 329. Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Woolley, J. D. (1982). Paradigms and processes in reading comprehension. Journal of experimental psychology: General, 111(2), 228. Kaan, E. (2007). Event‐related potentials and language processing: A brief overview. Language and linguistics compass, 1(6), 571-591. Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., & Holcomb, P. (2000). The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(2), 159-201. Kaan, E., & Swaab, T. Y. (2003). Repair, revision, and complexity in syntactic analysis: An electrophysiological differentiation [Article]. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 98-110. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903321107855 Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 133-156. Keyser, S. J., & Roeper, T. (1984). On the middle and ergative constructions in English. Linguistic inquiry, 15(3), 381-416. Kluender, R., & Kutas, M. (1993). Bridging the gap: Evidence from ERPs on the processing of unbounded dependencies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(2), 196-214. Koring, L., Mak, P., & Reuland, E. (2012). The time course of argument reactivation revealed: Using the visual world paradigm. Cognition, 123(3), 361-379. Kuperberg, G. R., Brothers, T., & Wlotko, E. W. (2020). A Tale of Two Positivities and the N400: Distinct Neural Signatures Are Evoked by Confirmed and Violated Predictions at Different Levels of Representation [Article]. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 32(1), 12-35. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01465 Kuperberg, G. R., Choi, A., Cohn, N., Paczynski, M., & Jackendoff, R. (2010). Electrophysiological correlates of complement coercion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 2685-2701. Kuperberg, G. R., Sitnikova, T., Caplan, D., & Holcomb, P. J. (2003). Electrophysiological distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(1), 117-129. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00086-7 Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203-205. Lee, C.-Y., Liu, Y.-N., & Tsai, J.-L. (2012). The time course of contextual effects on visual word recognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 285. Li, P., Bates, E., Liu, H., & MacWhinney, B. (1992). Cues as functional constraints on sentence processing in Chinese. In Advances in psychology (Vol. 90, pp. 207-234). Elsevier. Li, X., Zhao, H., & Lu, Y. (2014). The relation between thematic role computing and semantic relatedness processing during on-line sentence comprehension. PloS one, 9(4), e95834. Liao, C.-H., & Lau, E. (2020). Enough time to get results? An ERP investigation of prediction with complex events. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(9), 1162-1182. Liao, C. H., Lau, E., & Chow, W. Y. (2022). Towards a processing model for argument-verb computations in online sentence comprehension [Article]. Journal of Memory and Language, 126, 15, Article 104350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2022.104350 Lopez-Calderon, J., & Luck, S. J. (2014). ERPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event related potentials [Article]. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 14, Article 213. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213 MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological review, 101(4), 676. Oldfield, R. C. (1971). THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS: THE EDINBURGH INVENTORY [Article]. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(6), 785-806. Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P. J., & Swinney, D. A. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 20(4), 786. Perlmutter, D. M. (1978). Impersonal Passives and the Unaccusative Hypothesis. Proceedings of the annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 4, 157. https://doi.org/10.3765/bls.v4i0.2198 Schwarz, F., & Zehr, J. (2021). Tutorial: Introduction to PCIbex–An Open-Science Platform for Online Experiments: Design, Data-Collection and Code-Sharing. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Shetreet, E., Friedmann, N., & Hadar, U. (2010). The neural correlates of linguistic distinctions: Unaccusative and unergative verbs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(10), 2306-2315. Stroik, T. (1999). Middles and reflexivity. Linguistic inquiry, 30(1), 119-131. Sung, K.-M. (1994). Case assignment under incorporation. University of California, Los Angeles. Tao, Y. (2011). Chinese middle constructions: A case of disposition ascription. JASP Team. (2021). JASP (Version 0.16)[Computer software]. In https://jasp-stats.org/ Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (1990). Interactions between sentence context and word frequencyinevent-related brainpotentials. Memory & cognition, 18(4), 380-393. Van Valin Jr, R. D. (1980). On the distribution of passive and antipassive constructions in universal grammar. Lingua, 50(4), 303-327. Wang, L. M., Schlesewsky, M., Bickel, B., & Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. (2009). Exploring the nature of the 'subject'-preference: Evidence from the online comprehension of simple sentences in Mandarin Chinese. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(7-8), 1180-1226. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960802159937 Wu, S.-Y., & Lai, H.-L. (2022). EXPLORING NEW PASSIVE CONSTRUCTION" BEI+ X" FROM THE PERSPECTIVES OF EXEMPLAR MODEL AND LEXICAL-CONSTRUCTIONAL APPROACH. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics, 20(1). 王硯農. (1987). 汉语动词 : 结果补语搭配词典 (第1版 ed.). 北京语言学院. 陳明蕾, 王學誠, & 柯華葳. (2009). 中文語意空間建置及心理效度驗證: 以潛在語意分析技術為基礎. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 51(4), 415-435.
|