|
Adams, R.B. & Janata, P. (2002). A comparison of neural circuits underlying auditory and visual object categorization. Neuroimage, 16,361–377. Ädel, A. (2014). Metonymy in the semantic field of verbal communication: A corpus-based analysis of WORD. Journal of Pragmatics, 67, 72–88. Altarriba, J., Bauer, L.M., Benvenuti, C. (1999). Concreteness, Context Availability, and Imageability Ratings and word Associations for Abstract, Concrete, and Emotion Words. Behavioral Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 31(4), 578–602. Alloway, T.P., Gathercole, S.E., & Pickering, S.J. (2006). Verbal and visuo-spatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development, 77, 1698–1716. Ahrens, K., Liu, H. L., Lee, C.Y., Gong, S.P., Fang, S.Y., & Hsu, Y.Y. (2007). Functional MRI of conventional and anomalous metaphors in Mandarin Chinese. Brain Lang, 100, 163–171. Anaki, D., Faust, M., & Kravetz, S. (1998). Cerebral hemispheric asymmetries in processing lexical metaphors. Neuropsychologia, 36,353–362. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of learning and motivation, 8, 47-89. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in cognitive sciences, 4(11), 417–423. Bambini, V. (2010). Neuropragmatics: a foreword. Ital. J. Linguist, 22, 1–20. Bambini, V., Gentili, C., Ricciardi ,E., Bertinetto, P.M., & Pietrini, P. (2011). Decomposing metaphor processing at the cognitive and neural level through functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain. Res Bull, 86,203–216. Beeman, M. (1993). Semantic processing in the right hemisphere may contribute to drawing inferences from discourse. Brain Lang, 44,80–120. Beeman, M. (1998). Coarse semantic coding and discourse comprehension. In: Beeman, M., Chiarello, C. (Eds.) Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. pp 255–284. Beeman, M., Friedman, R.B., Grafman, J., Perez, E., Diamond, S., & Lindsay, M.B. (1994). Summation priming and coarse semantic coding in the right hemisphere. J Cogn Neurosci, 6, 26–45. Beeman, M., Bowden, E.M., & Gernsbacher, M.A. (2000). Right and left hemisphere cooperation for drawing predictive and coherence inferences during normal story comprehension. Brain Lang, 71, 310–336. Binder, J.R., Desai, R.H., Graves, W.W., & Conant, L.L. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. (2009). Cereb. Cortex, 19, 2767–2796. Bischofshausen, S., Makoid, L. A., & Cole, J. (1989) Effects of inference requirements on comprehension and recognition of metaphors. Metaphor and Symbol, 4(4), 227–246. Bluhm, R.L., Clark, C.R., McFarlane, A.C., Moores, K.A., Shaw, M.E., & Lanius, R.A. (2010). Default network connectivity during a working memory task. Hum Brain Mapp, 32(7), 1029–35. Blasko, D. G., & Kazmerski, V. A. (2006). ERP correlates of individual differences in the comprehension of nonliteral language. Metaphor and Symbol, 21(4), 267-284. Blankenship, A. (1938). Memory span: a review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 35(1), 1-25. doi:10.1037/h0061086. Bottini, G., Corcoran, R., Sterzi, R., Schenone, P. Scarpa, P., Frackowiak, R.S.J., & Frith, C.D. (1994). The role of the right hemisphere in the interpretation of figurative aspects of language: a positron emission tomography activation study. Brain 117, 1241–1253. Bohrn, T.C., Altmann, U., & Jacobs, A.M. (2012). Looking at the brains behind figurative language—A quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on metaphor, idiom, and irony processing, Neuropsychologia, 50 (11), 2669–2683. Botez-Marquard, T., Léveillé, J., & Botez, M.I. (1994). Neuropsychological functioning in unilateral cerebellar damage. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 21, 353–357. Bopp, K. L., & Verhaeghen, P. (2005). Aging and verbal memory span: A meta-analysis. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60B, 223– 233. Broadway, J. M., & Engle, R. W. (2010). Validating running memory span: Measurement of working memory capacity and links with fluid intelligence. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 563–570. Chen, Y. & Lai, H. (2013). Teaching English Idioms as Metaphors through Cognitive-Oriented Methods: A Case in an EFL Writing Class, English Language Teaching, 6 (6), 13–20. Carretti, B., Borella, E., Cornoldi, C., & De Beni, R. (2009). Role of working memory in explaining the performance of individuals with specific reading comprehension difficulties: a meta-analysis. Learning, and Individual Differences, 19, 246–251. Conway, A.R.A., Kane, M.J., Bunting, M.F., Hambrick, D.Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R.W. (2005). Working Memory Span Tasks: A Methodological Review and User's Guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 769–786. (http://www.pitt.edu/~tol7/res/research/psych-tests/rspan/) Davey, J., Thompson, H. E., Hallam, G., Karapanagiotidis, T., Murphy, C., De Caso, I., Krieger-Redwood, K., Bernhardt, B.C., Smallwood, J., & Jefferies, E. (2016). Exploring the role of the posterior middle temporal gyrus in semantic cognition: Integration of anterior temporal lobe with executive processes. Neuroimage, 137, 165–177. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.051. Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior, 19(4), 450-466. Démonet, J.S., Thierry, G., & Cardebat, D. (2005). Renewal of the neurophysiology of language: functional neuroimaging, Physiol. Rev. 85, 49–95. De Smet, H.J., Paquier, P., Verhoeven, J., & Mariën, P. (2013). The cerebellum: Its role in language and related cognitive and affective functions. Brain and Language. 127(3), 334-342. Den Heijer, T., Oudkerk, M., Koudstaal, P.J., Hofman, A., & Breteler, M.M. (2002). Hippocampal head size associated with verbal memory performance in nondemented elderly. Neuroimage. 17: 1365–1372. Engle, R.W., Tuholski, S.W., Laughlin, J.E., & Conway, A.R.A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory and general fluid intelligence: A latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331. Farmer, T.A., Fine, A.B., Misyak, J.B., Morten, M. & Christiansen, H. (2017). Reading span task performance, linguistic experience, and the processing of unexpected syntactic events, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(3), 413–433. Forgács, B., & Pléh, C. (2014). Figures of language in cognitive science in the light of figurative language processing in the brain. Hungary: Budapest University of Technology and Economics. Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The reading span test and its predictive power for reading comprehension ability. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 136–158. Gibbs, R. W., & O'Brien, J. E. (1990). Idioms and mental imagery: The metaphorical motivation for idiomatic meaning. Cognition, 36(1), 35–68. Glucksberg, S., Gildea, P., & Bookin, H. B. (1982). On understanding nonliteral speech: Can people ignore metaphors? Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior, 21(1), 85–98. Grahn, J., Parkinson, J.A., & Owen, A.M. (2009). The role of the basal ganglia in learning and memory: Neuropsychological Studies. Behavioral Brain Research, 199 (1), 55–60.
Haldane, M., Cunningham, G., Androutsos, C., & Frangou, S. (2008). Structural brain correlates of response inhibition in Bipolar Disorder I. J Psychopharmacol, 22(2),138–43. He, K. (2017). A Theory of Creative Thinking: Construction and Verification of the Dual Circulation model. Singapore: Springer. Health, S., McMahon, KL, Nickles, L, Angwin, A., Macdonald, AD, van Hees, S., Johnson, K, & McKinnon, E. (2012). Neural mechanisms underlying the facilitation of naming in aphasia using a semantic task: an fMRI study. BMC Neurosci, 13(1),98. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-98. Horga, G., Kaur, T., & Peterson, B. S. (2014). Annual Research Review: Current limitations and future directions in MRI studies of child- and adult-onset developmental psychopathologies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 55(6), 659–680. Jancke, L., & Shah, N.J. (2002). Does dichotic listening probe temporal lobe functions? Neurology. 58, 736–743. Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Psychological review, 99(1), 122. Jaeggi S. M., Buschkuehl M., Shah P., Jonides J. (2014). The role of individual differences in cognitive training and transfer. Journal of Memory and Cognition, 42, 464–480. Jung-Beeman, M., (2005). Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language, Trends Cogn. Sci., 9, 512–518. Kazmerski, V. A., Blasko, D. G., & Dessalegn, B. G. (2003). ERP and behavioral evidence of individual differences in metaphor comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 31(5), 673–689. Kutas, M., One lesson learned: frame language processing – literal and figurative – as a human brain function, (2006). Metaphor Symbol, 21, 285–325. Küper, M., Kaschani, P., Thürling, M., Stefanescu, M. R., Burciu, R. G., Göricke, S., Maderwald, S., Ladd, M. E., Hautze, H., & Timmann, D. (2016). Cerebellar fMRI Activation Increases with Increasing Working Memory Demands. Cerebellum. 15: 322. Lakoff, G. (1990). The Invariance Hypothesis: is abstract reason based on image-schemas? Cognitive Linguistics (includes Cognitive Linguistic Bibliography), 1(1), 39–74. Lai, V. T., van Dam, W., Conant, L. L., Binder, J. R., & Desai, R. H. (2015). Familiarity differentially affects right hemisphere contributions to processing metaphors and literals. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 44. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00044 Levorato, M. C., & Cacciari, C. (1992). Children's comprehension and production of idioms: the role of context and familiarity. Journal of Child Language, 19(2), 415–433. Leshikar, ED, Duarte, A., & Hertzog, C. (2012). Task-selective memory effects for successfully implemented encoding strategies. PLos OONE, 7(5). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038160. Linck, J. A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J. T., & Bunting, M. F. (2014). Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(4), 861–883. Littlemore, J. (2015). Metonymy: Hidden shortcuts in langue, thought, and communication. London: Cambridge University Press. Luis, E. O., Arrondo, G., Vidorreta, M., Martínez, M., Loayza, F., Fernández-Seara, M. A., & Pastor, M. A. (2015). Successful Working Memory Processes and Cerebellum in an Elderly Sample: A Neuropsychological and fMRI Study. PLoS ONE, 10(7). Marin, Blumenfield, & Kaushankaya (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 50 (4), 940- 967. Mashal, N, Faust, M, & Hendler, T. (2005). The role of the right hemisphere in processing nonsalient metaphorical meanings: Application of principal components analysis to fMRI data. Neuropsychologia, 43, 2084–2100. Mashal, N., Faust, M., Hendler, T., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2007). An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates underlying the processing of novel metaphoric expressions. Brain and Language, 100, 115–126. Mashal N., Faust M., Hendler T., & Jung-Beeman M. (2008). Hemispheric differences in processing the literal interpretation of idioms: Converging evidence from behavioral and fMRI studies. Cortex.,44, 848–860. Mashal, N., & Faust, M. (2008). Right hemisphere sensitivity to novel metaphoric relations: Application of the signal detection theory. Brain Lang, 104, 103–112. Mashal, N., Faust, M., Hendler, T., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2009). An fMRI study of processing novel metaphoric sentences. Laterality. 14, 30–54. Mashal, N. & Faust, M. (2009). Conventionalization of novel metaphors: A shift in hemispheric asymmetry. Laterality. 14, 573–589. McDermott, K.B., Petersen, S.E., Watson, J.M., & Ojemann, J.G. (2003). A procedure for identifying regions preferentially activated by attention to semantic and phonological relations using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia. 41, 293–303. Mechelli, A., Humphreys, G.W., Mayall, K., Olson, A., & Price, C.J. (2000). Differential effects of word length and visual contrast in the fusiform and lingual gyri during reading. Proc Biol Sci, 267 (1455), 1909-1913. Michels, L., Moazami-Goudarzi, M., Jeanmonod, D., & Sarntheina, J. (2008). EEG alpha distinguishes between cuneal and precuneal activation in working memory. NeuroImage. 40, 1296–1310. Milham, M.P., Banich, M.T., Webb, A., Barad, V., Cohen, N.J., Wszalek, T., & Kramer, A.F. (2001). The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res, 12, 467–473. Mason, R.A. & Just, M.A. (2004). How the brain processes causal inferences in text: A theoretical account of generation and integration component processes utilizing both cerebral hemispheres. Psychol Sci.15,1–7. Metcalfe, J. E., & Shimamura, A. P. (1994). Metacognition: Knowing about knowing. Massachusetts, MA: The MIT Press. Milosky, L., Wallach, G., & Butler, K. (1994). Nonliteral language abilities. Language learning disabilities in school-age children and adolescents, 275–303. Monetta, L., Grindrod, C. M., & Pell, M. D. (2008). Effects of working memory capacity on inference generation during story comprehension in adults with Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(5), 400–417. Marvel, C. L., & Desmond, J. E. (2010). Functional Topography of the Cerebellum in Verbal Working Memory. Neuropsychology Review, 20(3), 271–279. Na, D. G., Ryu, J. W., Byun, H. S., Choi, D. S., Lee, E. J., Chung, W. I., Cho, J.M., & Han, B. K. (2000). Functional MR Imaging of Working Memory in the Human Brain. Korean Journal of Radiology, 1(1), 19–24. Nagel, I.E., Preuschhof, C., Li, S-C, Nyberg, L., Bäckman, L., Lindenberger, U., & Heekeren, H.R. (2011). Load modulation of BOLD response and connectivity predicts working memory performance in younger and older adults. J Cogn Neurosci. 23:2030–2045. Olson, R.K. (1999). Reading Disability: Evidence for a Genetic Etiology. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 8, 52–55. Prat, C. S. (2011). The brain basis of individual differences in language comprehension abilities. Language and Linguistic Compass, 5(9), 635–649. Prat, C. S. & Just, M. A. (2011). Exploring the cortical dynamics underpinning individual differences in sentence comprehension. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 1747–1760. Prat, C. S., Mason, R. A., & Just, M. A. (2011). Individual differences in the neural basis of causal inferencing. Brain and Language, 116, 1–13. Prat, C. S., Mason, R. A., & Just, M. A. (2012). An fMRI Investigation of Analogical Mapping in Metaphor Comprehension: The Influence of Context and Individual Cognitive Capacities on Processing Demands. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(2), 282–294. Papagno, C., & Cacciari, C. (2010). The role of ambiguity in idiom comprehension: the case of a patient with a reversed concreteness effect. J. Neurolinguist, 23, 631–643. Piñango, M. M., Zhang, M., Foster-Hanson, E., Negishi, M., Lacadie, C., & Constable, T. R. (2017). Metonymy as Referential Dependency: Psycholinguistic and Neurolinguistic Arguments for a Unified Linguistic Treatment. Cognitive Science, 1–28. Qualls, C. D., & Harris, J. L. (2003). Age, Working Memory, Figurative Language Type, and Reading Ability Influencing Factors in African American Adults' Comprehension of Figurative Language. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12(1), 92–102. Rapp, A. M., Erb, M., Grodd, W., Bartels, M., & Market, K. (2011). Neural correlates of metonymy resolution, Brain and Language, 119 (3), 196–205. Rapp A., Leube D., Erb M., Grodd W., Tilo, & Kircher T. (2004). Neural correlates of metaphor processing. Cognitive Brain Research, 20 (3), 395–402. Rapp, A.M., Leube, D.T., Erb, M., Grodd, W., & Kircher, T. (2007). Laterality in metaphor processing: Lack of evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging for the right hemisphere theory. Brain Lang., 100, 142–149. Reid, G. (2012). The Routledge Companion to Dyslexia. London: Taylor & Francis. Rebollar, B. E. (2015). A relevance-theoretic perspective on metonymy. Social and Behavioral Sciences, 173, 191–198. Ruigrok, A. N. V., Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Lai, M.-C., Baron-Cohen, S., Lombardo, M. V., Tait, R. J., & Suckling, J. (2014). A meta-analysis of sex differences in human brain structure. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 39(100), 34–50. Salthouse, T. A. (1990). Working memory as a processing resource in cognitive aging. Developmental review, 10(1), 101–124. Salmon, E., Van der Linden, M., Collette, F., Delfiore, G., Maquet, P., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., & Franck, G. (1996). Regional brain activity during working memory tasks. Brain, 119(5), 1617–1625. Schmidt, G.L., Seger, C.A. (2009). Neural correlates of metaphor processing: The roles of figurativeness, familiarity and difficulty. Brain Cogn., 71, 375–386. Seger, C.A. & Cincotta, C.M. (2005). The roles of caudate nucleus in human classification learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(11), 2941–2951. Silani, G., Lamm, C., Ruff, C.C., & Singer, T. (2013). Right Supramarginal Gyrus Is Crucial to Overcome Emotional Egocentricity Bias in Social Judgments. Journal of Neuroscience, 33 (39),15466. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1488-13.2013. Stringaris, A. K., Medford, N. C., Giampetro, V., Brammer, M. J., & David, A. S. (2007). Deriving meaning: Distinct mechanisms for metaphoric, literal, and non-meaningful sentences. Brain and Language, 100, 150–162. Sugiura, M., Shah, N. J., Zilles, K., & Fink, G.R. (2005). Cortical representations of personally familiar objects and places: functional organization of the human posterior cingulate cortex. J Cogn Neurosci., 17, 183–198. Tourangeau, R., & Sternberg, R. J. (1981). Aptness in metaphor. Cognitive psychology, 13(1), 27–55. Tzou, Y-Z., Eslami, Z. R., Chen, H-C., & Vaid, J. (2011). Effect of language proficiency and degree of formal training in simultaneous interpreting on working memory and interpreting performance: Evidence from Mandarin–English speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism. 16(2), 213 – 227. Unsworth, N., Spillers, G. J., & Brewer, G. A. (2010). The contributions of primary and secondary memory to working memory capacity: An individual differences analysis of immediate free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 240–247. Ungerleider, L.G. & Haxby. J.V. (1994). “What” and “Where” in the human brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol., 4, 157–165. Van Dyke, J. A., Johns, C. L., & Kukona, A. (2014). Low working memory capacity is only spuriously related to poor reading comprehension. Cognition, 131(3), 373–403. Virshup, E. & Virshup, B. (1980). Visual imagery: The language of the right brain Imagery. Singapore: Springer. Westbury, C. F., Cribben, I., & Cummine, J. (2016). Imaging Imageability: Behavioral Effects and Neural Correlates of Its Interaction with Affect and Context. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 346. Yang, F.G., Edens, J., Simpson, C., Krawczyk, D.C. (2009). Differences in task demands influence the hemispheric lateralization and neural correlates of metaphor. Brain and Language, 111(2),114–124. Yang, F.G., Fuller, J., Khodaparast, N., Krawczyk, D.C. (2010). Figurative language processing after traumatic brain injury in adults: A preliminary study. Neuropsychologia, 48,1923–1929. Yu, Y., FitzGerald, T. H. B., & Friston, K. J. (2013). Working Memory and Anticipatory Set Modulate Midbrain and Putamen Activity. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(35), 14040–14047. Yang, J. (2014) The role of the right hemisphere in metaphor comprehension: a meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp, 35, 107–122. Zhang, S. & Li, C.S. (2012). Functional networks for cognitive control in a stop signal task: independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp, 33(1), 89–104. Zhou, H., Rossi, S., & Chen, B. (2017). Effects of Working Memory Capacity and Tasks in Processing L2 Complex Sentence: Evidence from Chinese-English Bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 595.
|